Dissociative amnesia

Dissociative amnesia

Everybody knows the term "amnesia". But what is it really about?


Amnesias are serious memory impairments. These disorders are caused by brain injury or disease, substance abuse, psychological or mixed mechanisms. There are studies highlighting patients with memory impairment after experiencing highly unpleasant or traumatic events. The traumatic event is poorly memorized and this impacts the memories preceding (retrograde) and following (antegrade) it. The term dissociative amnesia refers to a general memory disorder or a dissociative symptom of another disorder. Negative dissociative symptoms refer to a decrease in memory (amnesia, forgetfulness), while positive symptoms refer to an increase in memory (hypermnesia, precise memories).

Illustration - Amnesia (Source : lemonde.fr)


The term dissociative amnesia encompasses different disorders in terms of clinical and neuropsychological presentations, as well as many different definitions. Some people experience loss of personal information over several years of life (information retrieval is blocked). Other people may have amnesia involving only parts of the traumatic event (recording of information is incomplete due to emotional over-arousal). Usually, amnesia only affects the stressful time while leaving other aspects of their life intact. It is often claimed that these people can later regain their memory. This theory of dissociative amnesia remains highly controversial, with some authors suggesting it is common and others suggesting that it is devoid of evidence.



The main definition of dissociative disorders is related to trauma or stress. It is highlighted by the 10th edition of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10) which suggests that trauma or psychological stress are key players in the causes of dissociative amnesia. Although not an explicit diagnostic criterion in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th Edition (DSM-5), dissociative amnesia is listed alongside trauma and stress-related disorders. . There are two models that view retrograde dissociative amnesias as the result of recovering an impossible memory.

Figure 1
The four brain’s lobes including cerebellum and brainstem (Source : mybrainonline.ca)


In the first model, Kopelman postulated that psychological stress in combination with other psycho-socio-biological factors impacts the frontal executive system, which is located in the prefrontal lobe / cortex, causing this forgetfulness. In the second model, Markowitsch argues that in retrograde dissociative amnesia, these traumatic events cause the release of stress-related hormones resulting in what is called memory block syndrome. This syndrome is characterized by desynchronization of the frontotemporal regions during memory recovery (including the amygdala and hippocampus), especially in the right hemisphere. This model is supported by studies showing deregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in dissociative amnesia, which controls stress responses.

Figure 2
The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in response to a stressful event, with the impact of glucocorticoids (GC) on the frontotemporal regions.


Additionally, increased levels of acute stress hormones have been shown to affect the retention and retrieval of these memories.

Figure 3
Glucocorticoids increase the consolidation of memories but decrease their recovery. The effect of GC is modulated by norepinephrine (NE)
.


Finally, several studies that have used functional imaging methods, such as MRI, have provided evidence of metabolic or blood flow changes in areas involved in memory processing in these people, also known as survivors.


Indeed, the memory of trauma victims is disturbed in at least two ways. On one hand, survivors have intrusive memories of the traumatic event in which vivid and repeated sensory impressions and disturbing emotions associated with the event return. On the other hand, they have difficulty remembering important parts of the event. These features are not just observations made by Post Traumatic Stress Syndrome (PTSD) researchers. They are also included as symptoms of PTSD in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th Edition.


In light of the current results, how do you explain that some patients with PTSD have difficulty accessing important aspects of their trauma while at the same time suffering from repetitive intrusive memories of isolated details? The most likely explanation, according to Williams, comes from reduced executive processes (as in depression), in part due to rumination (brooding) and worry or attempts to avoid painful memories. These processes are vital because they make it possible to break habits, make decisions and assess risks, plan for the future, prioritize actions and deal with new situations. At the same time, recent findings with depressed individuals suggest that this forgetfulness does not generalize to intrusive reminders because involuntary recollection involves less executive surveillance. Thus, a patient with PTSD with reduced executive functions may find it difficult to voluntarily access details preceding the event, including (but not limited to) those of the traumatic experience, while still experiencing distress and boasting involuntary memories of these details.

Figure 4
Williams' CaR-FA-X model = CaR - Capture and rumination, FA - Functional Avoidance, and weakened executive control capacities (X). Three-factor model describing the mechanisms underlying the recovery of memories (overgeneral autobiographical memory) and prematurely interrupting the process of memory research (Source: researchgate.net)


It is good to know that there are solutions. To recover memory, for example, it is important to have a safe and supportive environment. This measure alone frequently leads to a gradual recovery of missing memories. If this is not enough, hypnosis or a semi-hypnotic state induced by adjunct therapy can aid in recovery. Psychotherapy, on the other hand, helps to deal with problems associated with recovered memories of traumatic or stressful events. Once the amnesia is lifted, this support makes it possible to make sense of the trauma or the underlying conflict, to solve problems associated with the amnesia episode, and above all, to allow patients to live their lives more serenely.


Sources :


1. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Publisher.


2. Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2013). Involuntary Memories and Dissociative Amnesia. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 174–186. doi:10.1177/2167702613496241 


3. Berntsen, D., Staugaard, S. R., & Sørensen, L. M. T. (2013). Why am I remembering this now? Predicting the occurrence of involuntary (spontaneous) episodic memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology : General, 142, 426–444.


4. Brewin, C. R., & Holmes, E. A. (2003). Psychological theories of posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 339–376.


5. Christianson, S.-Å., & Nilsson, L.-G. (1984). Functional amnesia as induced by a psychological trauma. Memory & Cognition, 12(2), 142–155. doi:10.3758/bf03198428


6. De Quervain, D. J.-F., Aerni, A., Schelling, G., & Roozendaal, B. (2009). Glucocorticoids and the regulation of memory in health and disease. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 30(3), 358–370.


7. Dalgleish, T. (2004). Cognitive approaches to posttraumatic stress disorder: The evolution of multi-representational theorizing. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 228–260.


8. Horowitz, M. (1986). Stress response syndromes. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.


9. ICD-10. Classification of mental and behavioral disorders: clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization, 1992.


10. Janoff-Bulman, R. (1989). Assumptive worlds and the stress of traumatic events: Application of the schema construct. Social Cognition, 7, 113-136.


11. Kopelman MD. Focal retrograde amnesia and the attribution of causality: an exceptionally critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2000; 17: 585–621.


12. Markowitsch HJ, Staniloiu A. (2013). The impairment of recollection in functional amnesic states. Cortex, 49: 1494–510.


13. Staniloiu, A., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2014). Dissociative amnesia. The Lancet Psychiatry, 1(3), 226–241. doi:10.1016/s2215-0366(14)70279-2 


14. Tramoni, E., Aubert-Khalfa, S., Guye, M., Ranjeva, J.P., Felician, O., Ceccaldi, M. (2009). Hypo-retrieval and hyper-suppression mechanisms in functional amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 47: 611–24.


15. Watson, L. A., Berntsen, D., Kuyken, W., & Watkins, E. R. (2013). Involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memory specificity as a function of depression. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 44, 7–13.


16. Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Herman, D., Raes, F., Watkins, E., & Dalgleish, T. (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 122–148.



17. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Bc5Z-Ra5Uf8


18. https://www.lemonde.fr/blog/realitesbiomedicales/tag/amnesie-retrograde/


19. http://mybrainonline.ca/?page=3


20.https://www.researchgate.net/figure/The-CaR-FA-X-model-Three-factors-CApture-Rumination-Functional-Avoidance-and-impaired_fig1_278411233


 


Comment ( 0 ) :
November 17, 2020
Authors
Share
Categories

    You might be interested in this:

    Subscribe to our newsletter

    We post content regularly, stay up to date by subscribing to our newsletter.